Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com

mystery of yawning 

 

 

haut de page

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

mise à jour du
30 juin 2022
Animals (Basel)
2022 May 26;12(11):1357
No Evidence for Contagious Yawning in Juvenile Ravens
(Corvus corax): An Observational Study  
Gallup AC, Schild AB, Ühlein MA, Bugnyar T, Massen JJM 
Psychology Department, State University of New York at Oneonta, New York, USA  

Chat-logomini

 Tous les articles sur la contagion du bâillement
All articles about contagious yawning
 
The overt and reflexive matching of behaviors among conspecifics has been observed in a growing number of social vertebrates, including avian species. In general, behavioral contagion&emdash;such as the spread of yawning&emdash;may serve important functions in group synchronization and vigilance behavior. Here, we performed an exploratory study to investigate yawn contagion among 10 captive juvenile ravens (Corvus corax), across two groups. Using observational methods, we also examined the contagiousness of three other distinct behaviors: stretching, scratching, and shaking. A total of 44 20 min observations were made across both groups, including 28 in the morning and 16 in the afternoon. The time and occurrence of all the behaviors from each bird were coded, and the temporal pattern of each behavior across both groups was then analyzed to assess the degree of social contagion. Overall, we found no evidence for contagious yawning, stretching, scratching, or shaking. However, yawns were relatively infrequent per observation (0.052 ± 0.076 yawns/bird) and thus experimental methods should be used to support this finding.
 
 
La correspondance manifeste et réflexive des comportements entre congénères a été observée chez un nombre croissant de vertébrés sociaux, y compris chez les espèces aviaires. En général, la contagion comportementale - telle que la propagation des bâillements - peut remplir des fonctions importantes dans la synchronisation des groupes et le comportement de vigilance. Ici, nous avons réalisé une étude exploratoire pour étudier la contagion des bâillements chez 10 corbeaux juvéniles en captivité (Corvus corax), dans deux groupes. En utilisant des méthodes d'observation, nous avons également examiné la contagiosité de trois autres comportements distincts : l'étirement, le grattage et le tremblement. Un total de 44 observations de 20 minutes a été réalisé dans les deux groupes, dont 28 le matin et 16 l'après-midi. L'heure et l'occurrence de tous les comportements de chaque oiseau ont été codées, et le schéma temporel de chaque comportement dans les deux groupes a ensuite été analysé pour évaluer le degré de contagion sociale. Globalement, nous n'avons trouvé aucune preuve de contagion des bâillements, des étirements, des grattages ou des secousses. Cependant, les bâillements étaient relativement peu fréquents par observation (0,052 ± 0,076 bâillements/oiseau) et des méthodes expérimentales devraient donc être utilisées pour étayer cette constatation.
Introduction
The overt and reflexive matching of behaviors among conspecifics, also referred to as behavioral contagion [1], is common among social species and could provide fitness advantages to group members [2]. In particular, contagious behaviors may serve important functions in synchronizing activity patterns and facilitating collective vigilance within groups [3,4,5]. Although the study of contagious behaviors has focused primarily on mammalian species [6,7,8,9,10], a number of studies have also found evidence for behavioral contagion among birds [11,12,13,14,15,16].
 
Perhaps the exemplar of contagious behavior is yawning. In humans, seeing, hearing, or even thinking about others yawning triggers contagion [17,18,19]. Among non-human primates, experimental evidence for contagious yawning has been documented in chimpanzees [6,20,21], bonobos [22], and orangutans [23]. Experimental studies have also shown evidence for yawn contagion in a subline of high-yawning Sprague Dawley rats [24] as well in as domesticated dogs in response to human yawns [25], but not to conspecifics [26]. Yawn contagion has been further reported within observational studies of gelada baboons [27], captive wolves [28], domesticated pigs [29], and African lions [3]. Limited evidence for contagious yawning has also been documented in African elephants [30], southern elephant seals [31], and domesticated sheep [32]. Some other mammalian species that have been studied for yawn contagion, but have revealed no evidence for this effect, include common marmosets, ring-tailed and ruffed lemurs, as well as lowland gorillas [33,34,35].
 
To date, the only evidence for contagious yawning in a non-mammalian species is in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). An initial observational study found that yawns were temporally clustered in a captive flock, and that the clumping of yawns could not be explained by circadian factors [36]. A subsequent study confirmed the presence of yawn contagion in this species, using experimental manipulations which included both live interactions and video recordings of conspecifics [37]. To our knowledge, no other species of bird has been tested for contagious yawning; thus, it remains likely that yawn contagion is also prevalent within this taxon.
One avian species that has been of particular interest in behavioral research is common ravens (Corvus corax). Ravens are a moderately social species, with non-breeders regularly forming groups during foraging and roosting [38,39]. These birds remain keenly attuned to the behaviors of group members [40], and previous studies have shown that they display collective behaviors in flock formation [41] and feeding recruitment [42], can cooperate extensively [43], and are able to coordinate the necessary actions for cooperation [44]. Moreover, recent studies have shown that ravens synchronize their play behavior [45] and display contagious allopreening [46]. However, some of the more prototypical contagious behaviors, such as yawning, have yet to be examined in this species. Ravens represent a good candidate for the study of contagious yawning, given that this response has been linked with empathy and emotional contagion [7,8,47,48], and recent studies have shown that these birds display both positive and negative forms of emotional contagion [49,50]. Similarly, ravens show complex social cognition [38,40,51,52], and some of their socio-cognitive skills, such as consolation and emotional contagion [49,53], seem to be linked to empathy, to some degree [28,54].
 
Therefore, the current study sought to investigate the presence of contagious yawning in captive groups of juvenile common ravens. Raven juveniles spend the first years of their lives in flocks, before they might establish a pair bond and acquire a territory. The juvenile period is, in fact, the most social period for ravens [39], and in these flocks, they form multiple differential social bonds. Thus, during this developmental stage, these birds would specifically benefit from any advantages related to behavioral contagion, making the animals selected for this study the best sample for examining contagion.
 
In addition to yawning, the contagiousness of the following three behaviors was also examined: stretching, scratching, and shaking. Stretching is often associated with yawning across species [55,56] and, in birds, may function in promoting preparation for flight. In addition, similar to yawning, stretching has previously been shown to be contagious among budgerigars [36,57,58]. Scratching is another behavior known to be contagious both in humans [59] and other mammalian species [31,60,61] but, to date, has not been examined among birds. Similar to stretching, contagious scratching has previously been studied alongside yawn contagion [62]. In addition, mirror neurons have also been implicated in both responses [63,64]. Lastly, shaking behavior, representing a conspicuous shuttering of the feathers, was examined, due to its common occurrence and potential links to arousal and group activity [65].
 
Using observational methods from previous studies of behavioral contagion in budgerigars [36] and marmosets [33], we examined the temporal distribution of each of these four behaviors to test for the presence of non-random clustering or clumping through behavioral runs that would be indicative of contagion. In addition to assessing the social influence on these responses, the naturalistic frequency and circadian variation of these four behaviors was examined for the first time.
 
 
Discussion
This study represents the second attempt to measure contagious yawning and stretching, and the first attempt to measure contagious scratching and shaking, in a species of bird. Despite prior studies reporting various forms of behavioral contagion among ravens [45,46], the current study did not find evidence for contagious yawning, stretching, scratching, or shaking in this species. While each behavior was significantly clustered in time for at least one of the observations, combined probability analyses (taking into account the probability values from across all observations) definitively revealed no overall effect of contagion. This was true when examining both the distribution at 30 s and at the less conservative 60 s bins (see Supplementary Materials).
 
In addition to addressing behavioral contagion, the current findings also provide the first account of the naturalistic frequency of these behaviors in this species, albeit among small groups in captivity. The observational data collected here suggest that in each hour, ravens yawn 1.6 times, stretch 10.7 times, scratch 12.2 times, and shake 20.3 times, on average. However, there was large individual variability in the expression of these behaviors (Table 2). Yawning and stretching occurred with relatively equal frequency in both the morning and afternoon hours, while scratching and shaking were both more common in the afternoon. In comparison to the budgerigar, in which there is comparable avian data for the relative frequencies of yawning and stretching [36], the rate of stretching for ravens was highly similar, while yawns were only about half as frequent. In fact, the majority of the birds (6/10) in the current study did not yawn a single time across the 44 observations. Additionally, budgerigars displayed an increase in yawn frequency as the day progressed [36], while there was no difference in the frequency of yawning among ravens between the morning and afternoon observations. Whether ravens truly deviate from the pattern observed in budgerigars would require a better investigation of ravens' activity patterns, particularly since the current study did not encompass many observations in the afternoon, or any between 11:30 and 15:00.
 
The absence of yawn and stretch contagion in ravens is also in contrast to observational and experimental studies in budgerigars [36,37]. However, potential comparative differences in these responses are to be expected, based on ecological factors and evolutionary history [5]. While ravens are highly gregarious and possess sophisticated social cognition [38,39,52], they live in much smaller groups composed of pair bonds and display less collective behavior in flocking, compared to budgerigars [71]. Given that contagious yawning and stretching are thought to promote motor synchrony [3] and collective vigilance [57,58], this could explain the difference between the two species. Nevertheless, ravens do tend to cooperate in these small parties when scavenging on large prey that is monopolized by pair-bonded individuals or large predators [72], which does require coordination and vigilance that may be enhanced by contagious yawning. Additionally, the social structure of ravens, with their fission&endash;fusion spatial and temporal dynamics [39,73], does resemble that of chimpanzees [74], which do show contagious yawning [8,20,21].
 
Together, these conflicting comparative findings cast doubt on the purported link between contagious yawning and emotional contagion [8] and suggest that these processes are independent. While some experimental studies have reported emotional contagion among ravens [49,50], to date, there is no evidence for this capacity among budgerigars. Instead, contagious yawning may be tied to bodily synchrony only [75], which budgerigars display when interacting with conspecifics [76].
 
Given the inherent limitations of observation research, experimental methods should be performed in the future to support the null findings for contagion effects. In particular, the overall occurrence of yawning was quite low, limiting the ability to effectively analyze the social influence of this response. Generally, we cannot rule out that our sample size, though large by the standards of ravens in captivity, was too small to detect a significant effect. To a lesser extent, the same issue could have applied to all the other behaviors as well (though the frequencies for these were much higher). Future experimental research could also examine different time scales at which subsequent behaviors should be considered contagious. Due to the outdoor aviary, there were influential factors to be considered: throughout the observational study, the temperature was not constant, and this is known to influence yawning in birds [77,78]. Parasite load could also have had an influence on scratching levels. Furthermore, the two compartments of the aviary were not completely visually nor acoustically separated; therefore, it is possible that adjacent behaviors between the groups could have occurred and gone unnoticed during the experimental sessions. These factors, however, were not likely to obfuscate our results significantly, since the ravens' visual access between compartments was still largely obscured.
 
Conclusions
Overall, this study represents the second attempt to measure contagious yawning in birds. The temporal analyses presented here do not suggest the presence of contagious yawning, nor any of the other behaviors measured. Given the low frequency of yawning and the limitations of observation research, experimental setups are needed to confirm and clarify these findings, i.e., by using live birds or video recordings as a target stimulus. Nonetheless, this study revealed novel effects with respect to the naturalistic frequency and circadian variation of some everyday behaviors in juvenile common ravens.