Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com

mystery of yawning 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

haut de page

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

mise à jour du
19 août 2014
PeerJ
2014; 2:e519

 Yawn contagion in humans and bonobos:
emotional affinity matters more than species
Elisabetta Palagi, Ivan Norscia, Elisa Demuru
 
Natural History Museum, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies
Unit of Cognitive Primatology & Primate Center, CNR, Rome, Italy
Bioscience Department, University of Parma, Parma, Italy

Chat-logomini

Tous les articles sur la contagion du bâillement
All articles about contagious yawning
 
-Demuru E, Palagi E. In Bonobos Yawn Contagion Is Higher among Kin and Friends. PLoS One. 2012; 7(11): e49613
-Leone A, Mignini M, Mancini G, Palagi E. Aggression does not increase friendly contacts among bystanders in geladas (Theropithecus gelada) Primates. 2010;51(4):299-305
-Leone A, Ferrari PF, Palagi E. Different yawns, different functions? Testing social hypotheses on spontaneous yawning in Theropithecus gelada. Scientific Reports 2014;4;4010
-Norscia I, Palagi E. Yawn Contagion and Empathy in Homo sapiens. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(12): e28472
-Norscia I, Demuru E, Palagi E. She more than he: gender bias supports the empathic nature of yawn contagion in Homo sapiens. R. Soc. open sci. 2016:3:150459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150459
-Palagi E, Leone A, Mancini G, Ferrari PF. Contagious yawning in gelada baboons as a possible expression of empathy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106(46):19262-19267
-Palagi E, Norscia I, Demuru E. Yawn contagion in humans and bonobos: emotional affinity matters more than species Peer J 2:e519
-Palagi E, Norscia I, Cordoni G. Lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) failed to respond to others' yawn: Experimental and naturalistic evidence. J Comp Psychol. 2019
-Palagi E, Guillén-Salazar F, Llamazares-Martín C. Spontaneous Yawning and its Potential Functions in South American Sea Lions (Otaria flavescens). Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):17226. cia I, Demuru E. Yawn contagion in humans and bonobos: emotional affinity matters more than species PeerJ 2:e519
-Palagi E, Cordoni G. Intraspecific Motor and Emotional Alignment in Dogs and Wolves: The Basic Building Blocks of Dog&endash;Human Affective Connectedness. Animals 2020;10(2):241
-Norscia I, Zanoli A, Gamba M, Palagi E. Auditory Contagious Yawning Is Highest Between Friends and Family Members: Support to the Emotional Bias Hypothesis. Front Psychol. 2020 Apr 3;11:442
-Tan J, Ariely D, Hare B. Bonobos respond prosocially toward members of other groups. Scientific Reports 2017;7:n° 14733
-Zannella A, Stanyon R, Palagi E. Yawning and Social Styles: Different Functions in Tolerant and Despotic Macaques (Macaca tonkeana and Macaca fuscata). J Comp Psychol. 2017
-Zannella A, Norscia I, Stanyon R, Palagi E. Testing Yawning Hypotheses in Wild Populations of Two Strepsirrhine Species: Propithecus Verreauxi and Lemur Catta. Am J Primatol. 2015;77(11):1207-1215
 
 Introduction
 
Most behavioral and cognitive studies have used the most sophisticated human abilities (e.g., theory of mind and language) as the main ground to make comparative arguments. Yet, this top-down approach has contributed to drawing the line between human and other animals' skills. Indeed, selecting the emotional and cognitive "pinnacles of mental evolution" the human brain (sensu de Waal & Ferrari, 2010, p. 201) can shed light on unique human traits but not on the common basic mechanisms underlying the more complex ones (de Waal & Ferrari, 2010; Hecht et al., 2012). While the hominin fossil record cannot give us any clues on the social and emotional abilities that may have paved the way for the emergence of human empathy, studying synchronous behaviours and body resonance (e.g., laughing, rapid facial mimicry, yawning; Provine, 2012; Davila-Ross, Menzler & Zimmermann, 2008; Mancini, Ferrari & Palagi, 2013a) in our closest living relatives may provide valuable information about how empathy evolved.
 
Empathy is a multilayered phenomenon, whose shared basal forms represent the pre-existing framework in which more complex emotional and cognitive processes are nestled (Preston & de Waal, 2002; de Waal, 2008). Perceiving and sharing others' emotions, the so called "affective empathy", is a phylogenetically old capacity. Affective empathy is an automatic process and may come about through emotional contagion (de Waal, 2008). At a higher layer of the empathic sphere, we find "cognitive empathy" which requires self-other differentiation, perspective-taking and mental state attribution, basic capacities to infer others' emotional states. Affective and cognitive empathy dissociate in humans with the former preceding the latter both ontogenetically and phylogenetically (Decety, 2010). Humans show the most complex form of empathy and, therefore, are commonly deemed as the most empathic species (Preston & de Waal, 2002; Decety & Jackson, 2004; Decety & Cowell, 2014).
 
However, no quantitative, direct inter-specific comparison has ever been made to actually demonstrate that this assumption is true at each floor of the empathic building. Recent findings endorse that other animals show affective empathy (Palagi et al., 2009; de Waal, 2012a; Panksepp & Panksepp, 2013). In this view, cross-species research is needed to explore empathy capacities as a bottom-up, emotional and developmental process of the brain (de Waal, 2012b). According to several neurobiological (Cooper et al., 2012; Haker et al., 2013), psychological (Lehmann, 1979; Platek et al., 2003) and ethological findings (Palagi et al., 2009; Campbell & de Waal, 2011; Campbell & de Waal, 2014; Romero, Konno & Hasegawa, 2013) yawn contagion is an empathy-related phenomenon. Specifically, yawn contagion is a form of emotional contagion, which represents the most basal layer of the empathic domain (e.g., see the "Russian Doll Model" in de Waal, 2008; Preston & de Waal, 2002; Hatfield, Rapson & Le, 2009). Several good reasons make the yawn and its contagion excellent phenomena to explore the evolutionary roots of empathy.
 
Due to its evolutionary antiquity, a yawn can be easily detected and quantified, appearing morphologically identical across many different vertebrate taxa and, consequently, withdrawing the risk of any subjective interpretation (Provine, 2005). Therefore, the plesiomorphic nature of yawning enables cross-species research (Baenninger, 1987; Baenninger, 1997; Deputte, 1994; Guggisberg et al., 2010). Yawning is automatic, unstoppable and easily recognisable also when people try to hide or inhibit it due to cultural constraints (Lehmann, 1979). These features make this behaviour an honest signal and, therefore, highly reliable (Provine, 1986).
 
Yawn contagion is obvious in human beings&emdash;about 50 percent respond to video stimuli of yawning faces (Provine, 1986)&emdash;and seems to be based on a perception-action mechanism (Preston & de Waal, 2002; de Waal, 2012c), which consists in the involuntary re-enactment of an observed facial expression and creates shared representations. Neurophysiological evidence of this coupling has derived from the discovery of mirror neurons (di Pellegrino et al., 1992), recently associated with yawn contagion (Cooper et al., 2012; Haker et al., 2013). Therefore, yawn contagion provides a "low-tech" but significant evidence of mirror-like phenomena. Mirror neurons, firstly described in the pre-frontal cortex of the rhesus macaque, fires when the subject either performs a motor action or observes the same action performed by another subject (Gallese et al., 1996; Ferrari et al., 2003). A mirror system, homologous to that of macaques, has been later discovered in humans (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). The mirror mechanism (or motor mimicry), not requiring any kind of conscious awareness, creates an emotional bridge and, thanks to it, two individuals can synchronize their affective states (Hatfield, Rapson & Le, 2009). This emotional bridge, in turn, fosters mirroring and triggers positive feedback which constitutes the core of social affinity. Such feedback is more easily activated when subjects sharing a strong empathic bonding are involved. While a positive attachment enhances body and emotional resonance (e.g., emotional reactions, unconscious mimicry, self-other overlap and shared representations), a negative attachment or a prejudice can inhibit or suppress it (Xu et al., 2009; Avenanti, Sirigu & Aglioti, 2010). In humans, when the attachment is profoundly negative (as in case of defectors, enemies, competitors) empathy can even turn into Schadenfreude that is feeling pleasure from the sufferings of others (de Waal, 2008; Decety & Cowell, 2014). This emotional circuitry, in its positive and negative form, has been found not only in humans (Singer et al., 2006; Pfeifer et al., 2008) but also in monkeys (Masserman, Wechkin & Terris, 1964; Palagi et al., 2009; Mancini, Ferrari & Palagi, 2013a; Mancini, Ferrari & Palagi, 2013b; Ferrari, Bonini & Fogassi, 2009; Paukner et al., 2009) and apes (Anderson, Myowa-Yamakoshi & Matsuzawa, 2004; Davila-Ross, Menzler & Zimmermann, 2008; Campbell & de Waal, 2011; Campbell & de Waal, 2014).
 
In humans (Homo sapiens) and one of its closest living phylogenetic relatives, the bonobo (Pan paniscus), yawn contagion is present and significantly affected by the emotional closeness linking the responder to the first yawner (the trigger). In the two species, the yawn response is most likely when the yawning stimulus comes from kin or friends (Norscia & Palagi, 2011; Demuru & Palagi, 2012). Since both in humans and bonobos yawn contagion is a socially modulated phenomenon, these species are good models to test some hypotheses on the evolutionary origins of the linkage between yawn contagion and empathy. The opportunity provided by yawn contagion to apply the same unit of measurement and identical operational definitions permits to directly compare human empathy with that of other species. Indeed, most of the studies on empathic capacities in Homo sapiens have been carried out through questionnaires used to measure self-reported scores of empathy. This approach bears the risk of overestimating the human empathic potential and leads to the unfeasibility of comparing it to that of other species (Lawrence et al., 2004).
 
As a whole, the possibility of employing a highly straightforward behaviour, such as yawning, to explore a highly complex phenomenon, such as empathy, represents a unique opportunity to investigate and compare empathy in human and non-human species (de Waal, 2012b).
 
Theoretically, humans could appear as the most empathic species not because they experience greater emotional contagion (affective empathy) but because they can better understand others' perspective and simulate others' emotional experiences (cognitive empathy). Indeed, empathy is a construct comprising dissociable components interacting and operating in parallel (Decety & Cowell, 2014). Cognitive empathy can be impaired in autistic subjects (Dziobek et al., 2008; Usui et al., 2013) whereas emotional empathy can be dramatically impaired in narcissistic individuals (Ritter et al., 2011), criminal psychopaths (Woodworth & Porter, 2002), rapists (Englander, 2007), and child molesters (Marshall, Hamilton & Fernandez, 2001). Yet, both emotional and cognitive empathy contribute to physiological human empathy (Cox et al., 2011; Decety & Cowell, 2014). Therefore, if humans are actually the most empathic species, they should possess the highest empathic response starting with emotional contagion, the ground level of affective empathy.
 
We tested this hypothesis by comparing our species with bonobos, which diverged from the human line about 5-7 mya (Fleagle, 2013). We expect that the frequency of yawn response is always higher in humans than in bonobos, whatever the relationship quality linking the responder to the trigger. Since the time elapsing from the yawn stimulus to the response can be reduced when the social bond is strong (Norscia & Palagi, 2011), we also expect that the emotional affinity linking the subjects affects, more in humans than in bonobos, the promptness of the yawn response.
 
See the video
 
 
Discussion
 
The inter-specific analysis of the overall frequency of yawn contagion did not reveal any difference between human and bonobo response susceptibility. The inter-species comparison revealed a higher promptness in humans than in bonobos, who showed a longer tail-effect in their response latency. However, both species concentrated their responses within the first minute after perceiving the stimulus yawn (Fig. 1). As yawn contagion is the expression of a mirror-like phenomenon (Cooper et al., 2012; Haker et al., 2013), our findings suggest that not only is yawn contagion well-rooted in the biology of bonobos and humans but also that the neural processes underlying and modulating yawn contagion might have been already present in the most recent common ancestor of the two species (5-7 mya, Fleagle, 2013). The similar sensitivity to others' yawns shown by the two species is probably due to similar selective pressures. In fact, both species are characterized by obligate gregariousness based on one of the most complex forms of social structure: the fission-fusion system (Aureli et al., 2008). Behavioural (Couzin, 2007) and emotional synchronization (S˙pinka, 2012) has been playing a pivotal role in favouring and shaping the evolutionary pathways leading to social living (Video S1). Synchronizing with others requires the ability to mirror their motor actions, to experience their emotional states and, consequently, to act in the appropriate manner. In this process familiarity makes the difference. Our intra-specific analysis demonstrated that yawn contagion is a socially modulated phenomenon, with strongly bonded subjects showing a higher susceptibility than weakly bonded subjects, thus confirming previous results (humans, Norscia & Palagi, 2011; bonobos, Demuru & Palagi, 2012). In this respect, the degree of contagion of around 50% reported by previous studies using videos of strangers' yawns as stimuli (Provine, 2005) may be a conservative estimate.
 
The social modulation of yawn contagion has been proven also in the congeneric species of bonobos, the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Chimpanzees are more susceptible to respond to videos showing yawns performed by in-group than by out-group members (Campbell & de Waal, 2011). This result is in line with a great body of research on human in-group/out-group bias that affects numerous empathy-driven behavioural and emotional reactions (Bourgeois & Hess, 2008; Xu et al., 2009; Avenanti, Sirigu & Aglioti, 2010). Recently, Campbell & de Waal (2014) also found that the rate of chimpanzee response to others' yawns was similar when the stimulus came from an in-group chimpanzee and from a human subject, thus demonstrating that the chimpanzee involuntary empathic response goes beyond the species boundary, as it occurs in humans (Phillips, 2009). Yet, no evidence of a social modulation in chimpanzee yawn contagion emerged as a function of the relationship quality shared between the responder and the familiar trigger (Massen, Vermunt & Sterck, 2012). Both neuroanatomical (Rilling et al., 2012) and behavioral features (Hare, Wobber & Wrangham, 2012) can account for the different effect of the relationship quality on chimpanzee and bonobo yawn contagion. Compared to chimpanzees, bonobos seem to show a different emotional and affective sensitivity (Furuichi, 2011; de Waal & Lanting, 1997; Hare et al., 2007) and a milder competitive propensity (Kano, 1992; Tan & Hare, 2013). Moreover, genetic findings revealed that a DNA segment regulating the responses to a hormone involved in social bonding (vasopressin) is present in humans and bonobos, but can be absent in chimpanzees (Hammock & Young, 2005). However, the different effects of the relationship quality on yawn contagion in the two Pan species has to be taken cautiously due to the different methodological approaches used to verify and quantify the phenomenon (video stimulus&emdash;Massen, Vermunt & Sterck, 2012; living stimulus&emdash;Demuru & Palagi, 2012).
 
At the inter-specific level, striking differences emerged in the social modulation of human and bonobo yawn contagion. While strong and weak relationships between individuals had to be assessed according to species-specific criteria (see Methods section), the previous works on bonobos (Demuru & Palagi, 2012) and humans (Norscia & Palagi, 2011) indicate that the strong-weak categorization produces a similar effect on yawn contagion in either species, with yawn contagion being highest between individuals sharing strong relationships. This result is confirmed in this study. Compared to bonobos, the human susceptibility (Fig. 2) and promptness (Fig. 3) to others' yawns were significantly more potentiated when kin and friends were involved. Humans' responses were more frequent and faster when the trigger and the responder shared a strong emotional bond. On the other hand, susceptibility and promptness incredibly overlapped between the two species when a strong emotional involvement between subjects was lacking, thus indicating that emotional contagion is not always highest in humans. It is worth remarking that these findings would have been impossible to detect if limiting the analysis to the intra-specific level.
 
What explains the difference in yawn contagion between species is the deepness of the emotional affinity linking the subjects. Compared to bonobos, humans show a different degree of sensitivity at the most basal layer of empathy, but only when they are strongly emotionally involved. Moreover, the latency of yawn contagion is socially modulated in humans (the stronger the emotional involvement, the faster the response) but not in bonobos. Therefore, the positive feedback linking emotional affinity and the mirroring process is more easily and rapidly activated in humans than in bonobos. Such over-activation explains not only the human potentiated yawning response, but also other kinds of unconscious mimicry response, such as happy, pain or angry facial expressions (Chiao et al., 2008). Minagawa-Kawai and coworkers (2009) investigated the response latency of smiles in mother-infant pairs and found neurobiological support for the "over-activation" hypothesis. The faster response was associated with an increased activation in the regions around the orbitofrontal cortex in mothers while viewing their own infant's smile compared to an unfamiliar infant's smile. In these mothers, specific neuronal regions involved in positive emotional regulation were activated by both viewing familiar and unfamiliar infants but the magnitude of activation was greater when affective attachment was involved. A similar neuro-ethological approach has never been applied to quantify the extent of the neural activation at the basis of the difference in yawn contagion latency as a function of the emotional closeness.
 
The higher human sensitiveness to yawns emitted by friends and kin can be explained by the peculiarity of the emotional attachment characterizing our species (Bowlby, 1969; Maestripieri, 2003). Compared to bonobos, the strong relationships established between humans are probably qualitatively different, because they are built upon more complex and sophisticated emotional foundations linked to cognition, memory, and memories. This interpretation is in line with findings endorsing that affective and cognitive empathy cooperate in modulating the phenomenon of yawn contagion in humans as it occurs for other kinds of unconsciously mimicked behaviours (Van Baaren et al., 2009). The idea that also the cognitive component of human empathy explains some features of yawn contagion is supported by the ontogenetic co-emergence of yawn contagion and theory of mind capacities in children (Anderson & Meno, 2003; Helt et al., 2010; Millen & Anderson, 2011) and the correlation of these two phenomena in adults (Platek et al., 2003). Therefore, it seems that in humans affective empathy is enhanced by the cognitive components concurring in awakening and nourishing the affective involvement characterizing strong relationships (Van Baaren et al., 2009).
 
The most intriguing and original result emerging from our cross-species analysis is the strong similarity between human and bonobo contagion susceptibility when the stimulus comes from a familiar, but weakly bonded subject. This finding indicates that the foundations of the empathic mechanism are alike in the two species. The absence of a strong emotional involvement seems to bring to light the most basic component of yawn contagion by removing the influence of the higher and more complex layers characterizing the cognitive sphere of human empathy (e.g., perspective taking, mental state attribution, theory of mind). As a whole, our cross-species approach supports the shared and multilayered architecture of animal empathy (Preston & de Waal, 2002; de Waal, 2008). Therefore, we cannot state that emotional contagion is always highest in humans, but only that they are able to create a unique and extremely intense form of emotional attachment, thanks to the complexity of the neural circuits linking social experiences and cognitive capacities. When such a pervasive attachment is missing, humans climb down from the highest step of the empathic podium to return to the understory layer which our species shares with other great apes.